- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Measuring adult romantic attachment: psychometric properties of the brief Spanish version of the experiences in close relationships
Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica volume 33, Article number: 9 (2020)
Abstract
The Experiences in Close Relationships (Brennan et al, Attachment theory and close relationships, 1998) questionnaire is one of the most widely used measures of adult romantic attachment. Despite the advantages of the ECR, the length of this measure may discourage its use in clinical and research contexts. Consequently, the goal of this study was to develop a brief Spanish version of the ECR questionnaire and to examine its psychometric properties when administered to six different Spanish-speaking samples from Chile. Confirmatory factor analyses replicated the two-dimensional structure of the ECR and its invariance across gender. Results also supported the reliability and concurrent validity of our brief Spanish version of the ECR (i.e., Spanish ECR-12), by its association with measures of emotion regulation, dyadic empathy, psychological distress and well-being, and relationship satisfaction. The Spanish ECR-12 can be used by researchers and clinical professionals as an abridged measure of adult attachment.
Introduction
Attachment theory articulates the universal human need to create close emotional bonds that can provide comfort and protection during times of stress and pain (Bowlby, 1982). According to this theory, the quality of interactions with one’s attachment figures in childhood impacts early internal representations of the self and others, which, in turn, influence affect, cognition, and behavior. These internal models can affect the quality of close relationships throughout the lifespan (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016).
Adults with greater attachment security tend to view themselves as worthy of love and feel comfortable depending on others, while adults with greater attachment insecurity tend to perceive themselves and/or others negatively (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). Insecure adult attachment can be conceptualized according to two dimensions known as attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). Attachment anxiety is characterized by the degree to which individual feel that they do not deserve affection, care, and protection, which could lead to chronic doubt about their standing in a relationship due to their persistent concern about being abandoned or rejected and their excessive distress when their partner is unresponsive (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). Attachment avoidance is characterized by the degree to which an individual considers others as unavailable in times of need, which could lead to exacerbated self-reliance, discomfort with closeness and self-disclosure, and difficulty in trusting others (Mikulincer, Shaver, & Slav, 2006). Based on this conceptualization, attachment insecurity is characterized by either higher levels of attachment anxiety and/or attachment avoidance.
It is well documented that insecure romantic attachment is associated with poor outcomes in intrapersonal functioning and couple functioning (see Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016, for a review). In terms of intrapersonal functioning, attachment insecurity has been associated with mental health problems, including anxiety and depression (Zakalik & Wei, 2006), lower levels of life satisfaction (Guzmán-González et al., 2016a), and emotion regulation difficulties (Brenning & Braet, 2013; Guzmán-González, Lafontaine, & Levesque, 2016; Pascuzzo, Cyr, & Moss, 2013). In terms of couple functioning, attachment insecurity has been associated with lower levels of relationship satisfaction (Feeney, 2016; Li & Chan, 2012), both in heterosexual (e.g., Birnbaum, 2007; Butzer & Campbell, 2008; Guzmán & Contreras, 2012; Heresi-Milad, Rivera-Ottenberger, & Huepe-Artigas, 2014) and same-sex couples (Horne & Biss, 2009). Attachment insecurity has also been related to lower empathy for others (Wei, Liao, Ku, & Shaffer, 2011), including a romantic partner (Lafontaine, Guzmán-González, Péloquin, & Levesque, 2016; Péloquin, Lafontaine, & Brassard, 2011).
Measures of adult romantic attachment
Based on the seminal work of Hazan and Shaver (1987), who applied the principles of the attachment theory to romantic relationships, a vast number of self-report measures were developed, such as the Adult Attachment Questionnaire (Simpson, 1990), the Adult Attachment Scale (Collins & Read, 1990), and the Attachment Style Questionnaire (Feeney, Noller, & Hanrahan, 1994). Brennan et al. (1998) attempted a factor reduction of the various items available on all attachment measures in use at the time and identified two major factors common to most of these instruments, namely, attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety. Based on these results, they used the best 36 items to create the Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR; Brennan et al., 1998). To our knowledge, none of the questionnaires that were used to create the ECR has been adapted in Spanish, except for the Adult Attachment Scale (Fernández & Dufey, 2015).
Since the work led by Brennan et al. (1998), the two-dimensional conceptualization of adult attachment has progressively been the most recommended and prevalent way to capture variations in attachment over categorical classifications (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). Categorical measures are criticized due to a variety of reasons, including the fact that classifications are not stable over time, they are often treated as mutually exclusive, and they rely on the assumption that differences among people within a category are irrelevant (Gillath, Karantzas, & Fraley, 2016; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). The use of continuous measures is preferred because they address those limitations, capturing variability in each attachment dimension, leading to greater reliability.
Defended by Mikulincer and Shaver (2016), the ECR is the gold standard for measuring romantic attachment (i.e., attachment-related anxiety and avoidance) because of its good psychometric properties (i.e., high reliability and validity) and its validation for use in different cultures and languages. The Spanish version of the ECR has been validated in numerous Spanish-speaking populations (e.g., Alonso-Arbiol, Balluerka, & Shaver, 2007), including Chilean samples (Spencer, Guzmán, Fresno, & Ramos, 2013).
Despite the advantages of the ECR, the length of this measure (i.e., 36 items; 18 per dimension) may discourage its use in clinical and research contexts. An abridged version of the ECR would make it feasible to include the ECR in studies requiring a large battery of tests (Spencer, Alonso-Arbiol, & Fresno, 2013). To this day, there are two brief English versions of the ECR; both comprise 12 items. The ECR Scale Short Form (ECR-S; Wei, Russell, Mallinckrodt, & Vogel, 2007) is the result of six studies. The results of Wei et al.’s (2007) studies indicate that the ECR-S has good psychometrics properties (i.e., internal consistency, test-retest reliability, factor structure, and validity), comparable or equivalent to the original ECR. However, while Lafontaine et al. (2016a) acknowledged the contribution of ECR-S, they raised three limitations that justified the creation of a new abridged version of the original ECR. First, the ECR-S has reduced generalizability due to the characteristic of their sample (i.e., college students). Second, the factor structure is acceptable only after controlling for two additional latent variables accounting for response sets, which was not applicable to the original ECR. Third, they argued that the classical test theory, which was used to select the final scale items, has many statistical inconveniences. To overcome these difficulties, Lafontaine et al. (2016a) conducted five studies to create the ECR-12. Using item response theory, their studies demonstrated the good psychometric properties of the ECR-12 in different samples (i.e., students, community, clinical, and same-sex couples), preserving the good psychometrics properties of the original ECR and surpassing the ECR-S. Although two abridged English versions of the ECR exist, to our knowledge, there is only one brief Spanish version of the ECR (Chilean B-ECR; Spencer, Alonso-Arbiol et al., 2013a). Even though the Chilean B-ECR has good psychometric properties, it presents three limitations mentioned by the authors. First, the sample used is not generalizable to the entire population of Spanish-speaking individuals. Second, concurrent validity has not been evaluated. Third, this version is still lengthy (i.e., 25 items). Despite criticisms both on the extent and specificity of the evaluation of adult attachment (Fraley, Heffernan, Vicary, & Brumbaugh, 2011), the present study aims to provide a psychometrically sound abridged Spanish version of the ECR for the study of relationships in Spanish-speaking Chilean populations that has psychometric properties comparable to the original ECR (Brennan et al., 1998).
The present research
Given the importance of readily accessible brief scales of important constructs, such as romantic attachment, in different research contexts that are also suitable for a clinical setting, our general goal was to obtain an abridged Spanish version of the ECR that would be psychometrically sound for use with diverse Chilean populations. More specifically, our first goal was to test the factorial structure of the three existing brief versions of the ECR, given the lack of information available regarding the appropriateness of these versions for our data. The ECR-S (Wei et al., 2007), ECR-12 (Lafontaine et al., 2016a), and Chilean B-ECR (Spencer, Alonso-Arbiol et al., 2013a) proposed a two-factor model (i.e., attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance). We thus aim to test these three bidimensional structures on five independent samples of adults who had been involved in a couple relationship at least once in their lifetime: two student samples (samples 2 and 3), a community sample (sample 4), a community sample of gay and lesbian adults (sample 5), and a community sample of heterosexual couples (sample 6). As these three brief ECR versions may not be an appropriate representation of Chilean adults’ romantic attachment, our second goal was to develop a new brief version that would be comparable to the original ECR (Brennan et al., 1998), based on another large sample of Chilean adults from the community (sample 1). An exploratory factor analysis (EFA) approach was favored to select the best items assessing attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance on the existing 36-item Chilean ECR version (Spencer, Guzmán, et al., 2013b).
Our third goal was to examine the psychometric properties of the new brief version of the ECR scale, using all six samples of adults. Based on the results from Del Giudice’s (2011) meta-analysis, which suggested that men display higher avoidance and lower anxiety than women, we also examined possible gender differences in men and women’s responses (i.e., gender metric invariance) in our new brief Spanish ECR. The questions we anticipated to answer in the present study were as follows: RQ1: Will the new brief Spanish ECR be an appropriate representation of a population of Chilean adults involved in a couple relationship?; RQ2: Will the internal consistency of the new brief Spanish ECR be comparable with the 36-item Chilean ECR (Spencer, Guzmán, et al., 2013b?; RQ3: Will the romantic attachment dimensions of the brief Spanish ECR be correlated with the same dimensions of the 36-item Chilean ECR?; RQ4 to RQ7: Will there be an association between (a) emotion regulation difficulties, dyadic empathy, psychological distress, as well as individual and relationship well-being, and (b) attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance?
Method
Participants
We included a total of 3703 Spanish-speaking Chilean adults, coming from six independent samples. In addition to students (samples 2 and 3), adults from the community (samples 1 and 4), gay and lesbian adults (sample 5), and heterosexual couples from the community (sample 6) were included to increase the representability of our results and to validate the scale among several populations. Demographic characteristics of the six samples are presented in Table 1.
Procedure
The ethical components of the studies included here (or in the current manuscript) were approved by the Ethics Committee of the involved institutions. Participants from the six samples were recruited using a variety of methods, for example, snowball sampling, contacted by trained research assistants, and email listserv announcements. Data were collected in different cities in Chile (e.g., Antofagasta, Valparaíso, Santiago, Talca, Concepción). For all six samples, informed consent was obtained, and participants completed a paper-and-pencil version of the 36-item ECR Chilean version (Spencer, Guzmán, et al., 2013b) and additional validity scales assessing various attachment-related theoretical constructs. As six independent samples were used, some questionnaires were completed in some samples and not others; this is because the studies behind those samples were targeting different objectives.
Measures
All scales were chosen based on their psychometric properties and past empirical associations with attachment insecurities. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for these measures and their respective subscales are displayed in Table 1. Participants from all six samples completed a short demographic questionnaire, including items about their age, gender, and relationship status.
Emotion regulation difficulties
The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004), which was validated for use in a Chilean population by Guzmán-González, Trabucco, Urzúa, Garrido, and Leiva (2014), assesses emotion regulation difficulties. The DERS Chilean version comprises 25 items, which are evaluated on a 5-point scale, with responses ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). Examples of items are “I have difficulty making sense out of my feelings” and “I experience my emotions as overwhelming and out of control.” Higher scores indicate greater difficulties in emotional regulation. This instrument includes five subscales, with reported Cronbach's alpha coefficients ranging from .66 to .89. The five dimensions were averaged in a global score of emotional regulation. The DERS was used in samples 1, 2, and 5.
Dyadic empathy
The Interpersonal Reactivity Index for Couples (IRIC; Péloquin & Lafontaine, 2010), which was validated for use in a Chilean population by Guzmán-González, Péloquin, Lafontaine, Trabucco, and Urzúa (2014), assesses two aspects of dyadic empathy: perspective taking (e.g., “I sometimes try to understand my partner better by imagining how things look from his/her perspective”) and empathic concern (e.g., “I often have tender, concerned feelings for my partner when he/she is less fortunate than me”). The IRIC Chilean version comprises 13 items, which are evaluated on a 5-point scale, with responses ranging from 0 (does not describe me well) to 4 (describes me very well). All items are summed to obtain the global score. Higher scores indicate greater perspective taking and empathic concern. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients have been reported to be .58 and .82 for empathic concern and perspective taking subscales, respectively (Guzmán-González et al., 2014). This instrument was used in samples 1 and 2.
Psychological distress
Psychological distress was measured with three instruments:
The Outcome Questionnaire (OQ-45; Lambert et al., 1996), which was validated for use in a Chilean population (von Bergen & de la Parra, 2002), assesses outcome in psychotherapy in three areas: symptom distress, interpersonal relations, and social role. The OQ-45 comprises 45 items, which are evaluated on a 5-point scale, with responses ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (almost always). Examples of items are “I feel no interest in things” and “I feel lonely.” Higher scores indicate greater symptomatic discomfort. The validated Chilean version of the OQ-45 has excellent internal consistency, with Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of .91. This instrument was used in sample 1 as an indicator of psychopathology.
The Abbreviated Scales of Depression, Anxiety, and Stress (DASS-21; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995), which was validated for use in a Chilean population by Vinet, Rehbein, Román, and Saiz (2008), assesses three aspects of mental health: depression, anxiety, and stress. The DASS-21 comprises 21 items, which are evaluated on a 4-point scale, with responses ranging from 0 (it does not describe anything that occurred to me or that I felt during the week) to 3 (yes, it occurred to me a lot, or almost always). Examples of items are “I found it difficult to relax” and “I found it difficult to work up the initiative to do things.” Higher scores indicate more symptomatology. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients obtained by the authors of the validated Chilean version were .85, .83, and .73 for the depression, anxiety, and stress subscale, respectively. This instrument was used in sample 4.
The Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) Spanish version (Gempp, Avendaño, & Muñoz, 2004) assesses depressive symptoms in the general population. The CES-D comprises 20 items, which are evaluated on a 4-point scale, with responses ranging from 0 (rarely/never (1 day or less)) to 3 (most/all days (5–7 days)). Examples of items are “I felt that everything I did was an effort” and “My sleep was restless.” Higher scores indicate more depressive symptoms. The CES-D Spanish version has good internal consistency, with a Cronbach's alpha coefficient of .87. This instrument was used in sample 6.
Individual and relationship well-being
The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SLS; Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985), validated in Chile by Vera-Villarroel, Urzúa, Pavez, Celis-Atenas, and Silva (2012) assesses life satisfaction. The SLS comprises five items, which are evaluated on a 7-point Likert scale, with responses ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree). Example of items are “In most ways my life is close to my ideal” and “I am satisfied with my life.” Higher scores indicate greater satisfaction with life. This instrument has good internal consistency, with Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of .87 (Vera-Villarroel et al., 2012). This instrument was used in samples 4 and 5.
The Relationship Assessment Scale(RAS, Hendrick, 1988) Spanish version (Rivera, Cruz, & Muñoz, 2011) assesses relationship satisfaction. The RAS comprises seven items, which are evaluated on a 5-point scale, with responses ranging from 1 (low satisfaction) to 5 (high satisfaction). Examples of items are “How well does your partner meet your needs?” and “How good is your relationship compared to most?” In Chilean samples, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient is .71 (Rivera et al., 2011). This instrument was used in sample 6.
Data analysis strategy
First, confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) were conducted on samples 2, 3, 4, 5, and 6 to test the factorial structure of existing brief versions of the ECR. We tested the models separately for men and women from sample 6 to account for the non-independence of the dyadic data. Based on a meta-analysis suggesting a stable correlation between the ECR attachment anxiety and avoidance subscales (Cameron, Finnegan, & Morry, 2012), we estimated the covariance between the two factors. We tested each CFA using several fit indices, following the recommendation of Kline (2015). The comparative fit index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) values typically range from 0 to 1, and values equal to or greater than .90 are representative of an adequate fitting model (with values close to .95 or higher indicate a model that fits the data well). The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) calculates the average error of fit between the submitted model and the data, and a value equal to or lower than .08 corresponds to an adequate fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1993). The close fit hypothesis is supported if a 90% confidence interval includes the .05 value and the poor fit hypothesis is supported if the upper bound of the confidence interval does not exceed a .10 value (Kline, 2016). We used the χ2/df ratio because the chi-square test is perceived to be less valid with a larger sample size. A ratio value between 1 and 5 suggests a satisfactory fit between the proposed theoretical model and the data (Bollen, 1989).
Second, two-factor exploratory factor analyses (EFA) with maximum likelihood extraction and oblimin rotation were conducted on an independent sample (sample 1) to select the 12 best items for a psychometrically sound short version of the Spanish ECR. A Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) of .80 and higher indicated good sample adequacy and significant Bartlett’s test of sphericity supported the presence of correlations. Item selection was based on four criteria: (a) factor loadings, (b) item-total correlation, (c) low cross-loading (r < .30), and (d) content validity (i.e., covering the theoretical components of each attachment subscale).
Third, to assess the psychometric properties of the new brief Spanish ECR, CFA were conducted on samples 2 to 6, followed by gender metric invariance analyses (i.e., comparing the equivalence of factor loadings between men and women). Pearson correlation analyses were then conducted between the short and original version to assess the representativeness of our new subscales as well as Cronbach’s alpha coefficients, with 95% confidence intervals. Finally, we conducted multiple regression analyses on a series of validity criteria to examine the concurrent validity of the Spanish ECR-12. As recommended by Cameron et al. (2012), each analysis included both attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance as predictors to account for their shared variance. The same analyses were carried out with the respective original ECR subscale and t tests assessed the presence of significant differences between the standardized regression coefficients (original ECR vs Spanish ECR-12 subscales).
Results
Step 1: Testing the factorial structure of the existing brief versions of the ECR
Using samples 2 to 6, we conducted CFA to test the three existing brief versions of the ECR: the French/English ECR-12 (Lafontaine et al., 2016a), the English ECR-S (Wei et al., 2007), and Chilean B-ECR (Spencer et al., 2013a). The results (see Table 2) suggested poor fit for all three abridged versions, supporting our choice to select better items for a psychometrically sound new Brief Spanish ECR.
Step 2: Item selection process for the new brief Spanish ECR
Using a sample of 943 community-based Chilean adults (see sample 1 in Table 3), a two-factor EFA was conducted on the 36-item Chilean ECR version (Spencer, Guzmán, et al., 2013b). The KMO of .92 indicated good sample adequacy and Bartlett’s test of sphericity, χ2 (630, N = 943) = 12357.99, p < .001, supported the presence of correlations. The two-factor solution explained 36.43% of the shared variance from the 36 items. We then proceeded to select the best six items from the attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance subscale based on four criteria: (1) high factor loadings (see Table 4), (2) high item-total correlation (see Table 4), (3) low cross-loading (r < .30), and (4) content validity.
Based on these criteria, the six items selected for attachment anxiety were representative of its three theoretical components: fear of interpersonal rejection (items 6, 8), disproportionate need for approval from others (items 18, 20), and distress when one’s partner is unavailable (items 24, 30). Although item 32 presented high factor loading and item-total correlation, it was discarded because of its high correlation and similar wording with item 30. The six items selected for attachment avoidance were representative of two out of three theoretical components: fear of interpersonal intimacy (items 5, 13, 17, 23) and reluctance to self-disclose (items 9, 27). Excessive need for self-reliance, however, was not represented in the selected items.
A second two-factor EFA was conducted on the selected 12 items. The KMO of .87 indicated good sample adequacy and Bartlett’s test of sphericity, χ2 (66, N = 943) = 3424.29, p < .001, supported the presence of correlations. The two-factor solution explained 42.98% of the shared variance from the 12 items. As expected, attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance items showed high factor loadings (> .40) on their respective factors and low factor loadings on the other factor (< .20).
Step 3: Psychometric properties of the new ECR
Factorial structure
Confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) were conducted on samples 2 to 6 to confirm the factor structure of the Spanish ECR-12 across diverse Chilean populations. Maximum likelihood estimation was employed to estimate the two-factor model, with items 6, 8, 18, 20, 24, and 30 predicted by the Spanish ECR-12 anxiety latent factor and items 5, 9, 13, 17, 23, and 27 predicted by the Spanish ECR-12 avoidance latent factor. A covariance was also added between the error terms of two anxiety items (i.e., 6 and 8) to account for similar wording. As shown in Table 2, the indexes of fit were adequate for the Spanish ECR-12. The correlations between the two latent factors ranged from r = .24 (p < .001) in sample 5 to r = .42 (p < .001) in male partners from sample 6, suggesting a weak to moderate association. In sample 3, however, the correlation was null, r = .04 (p = .447).
Gender metric invariance
Comparison of the equivalence of factor loadings between men and women were conducted, using the metric invariance test on Mplus. Gender invariance testing revealed no significant difference between men and women in the factor loadings in sample 2, Δχ2(10, N = 919) = 10.59, p = .390; sample 3, Δχ2(10, N = 578) = 8.99, p = .532; sample 4, Δχ2(10, N = 436) = 7.65, p = .663; and sample 6, Δχ2(10, N = 180) = 14.93, p = .135. Gender invariance, however, was not obtained between gay and lesbian from sample 5, Δχ2(10, N = 467) = 27.87, p = .002. Further analysis revealed that partial metric invariance was supported; specifically, items 9 and 27 from the avoidance subscale showed higher factor loadings in gay men compared to women.
Correlations between the ECR subscales
To ensure the representativeness of the Spanish ECR-12, we conducted Pearson correlations with 95% confidence intervals between the brief and the original subscales of the 36-item Chilean ECR version (Spencer, Guzmán, et al., 2013b). The results, presented in Table 5, supported the presence of very high correlations between the brief attachment anxiety subscale and the original attachment anxiety subscale, with r coefficients ranging from .89 to .93. We also found very high correlations between the brief attachment avoidance subscale and the original attachment avoidance subscale, with r coefficients ranging from .85 to .92.
Internal consistency
Table 5 also presents Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of the Spanish ECR-12 and the original ECR (Brennan et al., 1998) subscales. Given that both of the Spanish ECR-12 subscales are comprised of a third (i.e., 6 out of 18) of the respective items in the original ECR, it is not surprising that the internal consistency Spanish ECR-12 subscales are systematically lower than those of the original ECR. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients varied from .72 to .83 for the brief anxiety subscale and from .78 to .89 for the brief avoidance subscale across samples, which suggested good internal consistency.
Evidence of concurrent validity
Table 1 displays the validity scales’ reliability coefficient and the standardized regression coefficients for all samples. As expected, both attachment-related anxiety and avoidance were associated with higher emotion regulation difficulties in community-based adult, student, and gay/lesbian adult samples. In samples 1 and 2, but not in sample 5, the Spanish ECR-12 subscale of anxiety appears to underestimate the association between this insecurity and emotion regulation difficulties, when its regression coefficients are compared with those of the original ECR anxiety subscale (ps < .01). As predicted, attachment-related avoidance was negatively associated with the two subscales of dyadic empathy in samples of adults from the community and students. In sample 1, but not sample 2, the Spanish ECR-12 subscale of avoidance appears to underestimate the association between this insecurity and perspective taking (i.e., cognitive empathy), when compared with the original ECR avoidance subscale (p < .01). Attachment-related anxiety was weakly related to lower perspective taking, but positively related to empathic concern (i.e., emotional empathy).
Both attachment-related anxiety and avoidance predicted higher levels of psychological distress (i.e., anxiety, depression, stress) and lower levels of life satisfaction across samples. In the community sample of couples, depression was more strongly related to attachment-related anxiety than avoidance for both men and women. No significant differences were found between the coefficients obtained with the original ECR and the Spanish ECR-12 for these measures, suggesting equivalence. Finally, relationship satisfaction was more strongly associated with attachment-related avoidance than anxiety in both samples 5 and 6.
Discussion
The desire to form close emotional bonds with others is a universal human need that reflects the profoundness of the effect of attachment in well-being (Bowlby, 1982). To examine the nature of emotional bonds, and its correlates on mental health and well-being (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016), it is necessary to have validated instruments that will assess this variable in different social and cultural contexts. The aim of the present study was to provide a significant contribution to the field by developing a new brief ECR (i.e., Spanish ECR-12) that would be appropriate to use with Chilean populations. This effort is considered relevant because validating the use of scores of one scale in a different context is always necessary, given that linguistic and cultural differences can be detected (Hambleton, Merenda, & Spielberg, 2005).
In that sense, the present work gives continuity to the study of adult romantic attachment in Latin American populations, with an instrument that overcomes the limitations of the previous short ECR versions. As demonstrated by the present study, the three existing ECR short versions were not suitable when applied to different Chilean samples because the factorial structure was not replicated. These results revealed that these versions would not be an appropriate representation of Chilean adults’ romantic attachment, supporting the aforementioned notion that the use of instruments in different contexts need to be empirically examined rather than assumed (Hambleton et al., 2005).
The appropriateness of this new measure was determined by examining the psychometric properties (i.e., factor structure, gender invariance, reliability, and concurrent validity) of the Spanish ECR-12 when administered to six samples of diverse Chilean populations. Our findings support the notion that the Spanish ECR-12 preserved the good psychometric properties of the 36-item Chilean ECR version (Spencer et al., 2013b) and the original ECR (Brennan et al., 1998).
Using statistical and theoretical criteria, six items from the original attachment anxiety subscale were selected (i.e., items 6, 8, 18, 20, 24, and 30) and six items from the original attachment avoidance subscale (i.e., items 5, 9, 13, 17, 23, and 27), preserving the two-dimensional structure of the original ECR (Brennan et al., 1998). These items are representative of all the theoretical components of attachment-related anxiety, but only two out of three components of attachment-related avoidance, as none of the retained Spanish ECR-12 item tapped the excessive need for self-reliance. It is possible that Chilean culture, characterized as a collectivistic society, is inconsistent with the reluctance to depend on others, a characteristic that is more frequently observed among individuals with greater attachment avoidance from more individualistic cultures (Friedman et al., 2010; Schmitt, 2010).
The Chilean culture may correspond to one of the types of collectivist cultures—the culture based on honor—characteristics of Mediterranean countries, belonging to Latin America, the Middle East, and Africa (Agishtein & Brumbaugh, 2013; Uskul, Oyserman, & Schwarz, 2010). These groups value having a good reputation and therefore a positive social image, which is maintained through social interdependence (Rodríguez-Mosquera, 2018).
The results of the CFA confirmed that the bidimensional structure of the Spanish ECR-12 was an adequate fit to the data in all five independent samples, with better indexes than the three existing brief versions. Our measure also proved to be invariant across gender. However, in the case of the lesbian and gay sample, two items from avoidance subscale were identified as not invariant (items 9 and 27). Both items refer to reluctance to self-disclose, being more representative of avoidance for gay men than lesbian. From a heteronormative point of view, gender shapes how men and women experience intimacy and self-disclosure of feelings toward the partner, with women expressing their personal feelings and their desire of intimacy more openly in their relationships (Elliott & Umberson, 2008; Peplau, 2001; Simon & Nath, 2004). Current knowledge about differences between gay men and lesbian in intimate relationships is still limited, but findings are in the same line: men in same-sex relationships are usually less likely to self-disclose and to communicate their feelings to their male partner than women are to their female partner (Peplau & Fingerhut, 2007; Umberson, Thomeer, & Lodge, 2015). And this aspect could be particularly salient among same-sex relationships, considering that communication has been identified as a central component of the relationship success among same-sex couples (Riggle, Rothblum, Rostosky, Clark, & Balsam, 2016). However, this is a tentative explanation that needs further exploration in future studies.
In addition, the Spanish ECR-12 proved to be a reliable measure in our six samples, with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from .72 to .83 for the attachment anxiety subscale and .78 to .89 for the attachment avoidance subscale. Although these coefficients were slightly lower than the alphas reported for the Chilean ECR version, this is an expected result given the reduction in the number of items.
Finally, attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance were consistently associated with several theoretically related variables (e.g., emotion regulation difficulties, dyadic empathy, psychological distress, life satisfaction, and relationship satisfaction), in a way that is in line with attachment theory and previous studies. Attachment anxiety encompasses a negative model of the self; this dimension is characterized by the fear of abandonment, dependence, and chronic doubts of one’s lovability, which is often associated with the hyperactivation of the attachment system, due to the dependency of others, and the excessive need for approval. Attachment avoidance encompasses a negative model of others; this dimension is characterized by discomfort with closeness, excessive self-reliance, and difficulty in trusting in others, which is often associated with the deactivation of the attachment system and expressed through their desire to physically and emotionally distance themselves from others (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). It is well established that attachment insecurity (i.e., higher attachment anxiety and/or higher attachment avoidance) is associated with poor outcomes for the individual and couple’s functioning (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). More specifically, and in line with our results, attachment insecurity has been associated with higher emotion regulation difficulties (Guzmán-González et al., 2016, b; Han & Kahn, 2017), lower empathy (Lafontaine et al., 2016, b), more mental health problems and psychological distress (Mortazavizadeh & Forstmeier, 2018), lower life satisfaction (Tepeli Temiz & Comert, 2018), and lower relationship satisfaction (Li & Chan, 2012; Feeney, 2016).
However, it is worth noting, that for two of the attachment-related constructs that we tested, links were found in some of our samples only. First, perspective taking was not associated with attachment anxiety in the community sample (sample 1), but we found a small association in sample 2 composed of university students; the effect sizes were small in both samples. More perspective taking in university students was associated with lower anxiety, which is theoretically expected and in line with findings from previous studies (Lafontaine et al., 2016a). Second, relationship satisfaction was slightly associated with both attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance within gay and lesbian individuals (sample 5). Based on the sample of heterosexual couples (sample 6), attachment avoidance was also related to relationship satisfaction, but not attachment anxiety, for both men and women. This last finding differs from previous evidence showing that attachment anxiety is related to lower relationship satisfaction (Feeney, 2016), but this is line with what has been detected in Spanish and Chilean heterosexual couples (Heresi-Milad et al., 2013; Molero, Shaver, Fernández, Alonso-Arbiol, & Recio, 2016). This result can be explained due to the small sample size of the heterosexual couple sample or to the greater acceptance of attachment anxiety in this cultural context (Molero, Shaver, Ferrer, Cuadrado, & Alonso-Arbiol, 2010), demonstrating a less harmful effect on relationship satisfaction than attachment avoidance. In brief, attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance were generally related to attachment-related constructs in the expected way. However, in some samples, some associations were stronger (or not significant) than in other samples for these two constructs. These results demonstrate the importance of distinguishing between different samples and highlighting the effect of other relevant variables (e.g., age, status relationship) in future research.
The findings of this study can broaden the discussion of cultural aspects related to the evaluation of the attachment construct in diverse Spanish-speaking populations, and also contributes to the existing instruments available in this context (e.g., Valor-Segura, Expósito, & Moya, 2009). Although the relevance of the construct in different cultures is recognized, it is necessary to take into account that its expression may vary in different cultures and groups (e.g., LGBT), and that some of its dimensions may be underrepresented (Daouk-Öyry & Zeinoun, 2017). Theoretical and empirical development is needed to better understand the cultural and contextual aspects of attachment, as these aspects are scarcely addressed in a systematic way (Keller, 2013; Vicedo, 2017).
Limitations of the present study include the sole use of self-report measures; other measures such as behavioral observations could be included in future studies. In addition, the use of different methods of sample recruitment (e.g., snowball sampling, contacted by research assistants or email) and their potential effects on the results was not possible to explore in the present study. Recruitment strategies could be included as a confounding variable in future studies. This study included many validity criteria variables such as relationship satisfaction, emotional regulation, and psychological distress. However, to provide more evidence of concurrent validity, future studies could consider other relevant variables related to couple functioning (e.g., commitment and dependence toward the partner) that were not included in the present study.
Conclusion
Despite its limitations, our findings supported the good psychometric properties of the Spanish ECR-12 across six diverse samples, such as university students, couples from the community, among others. In terms of its implications, the current study provides a suitable instrument that can be used by professionals or clinical working with individuals, couples, and families. In addition, this brief version can be a feasible tool for researchers conducting studies that require a large battery of test.
Availability of data and materials
The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available because it could compromise research participant consent, but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- ECR:
-
Experiences in Close Relationships
- Spanish ECR-12:
-
Spanish Experiences in Close Relationships 12 items
- ECR-S:
-
Experiences in Close Relationships Scale Short Form
- ECR-12:
-
Experiences in Close Relationships 12 items
- Chilean B-ECR:
-
Chilean brief version of the Experiences in Close Relationships
- EFA:
-
Exploratory factor analysis
- RQ1:
-
Research question 1
- RQ2:
-
Research question 2
- RQ3:
-
Research question 3
- RQ4:
-
Research question 4
- RQ5:
-
Research question 5
- RQ6:
-
Research question 6
- RQ7:
-
Research question 7
- RQ8:
-
Research question 8
- DERS:
-
Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale
- IRIC:
-
Interpersonal Reactivity Index for Couples
- OQ-45:
-
Outcome Questionnaire
- DASS-21:
-
Abbreviated Scales of Depression, Anxiety, and Stress
- CES-D:
-
Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale
- RAS:
-
Relationship Assessment Scale
- KMO:
-
Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin
- CFA:
-
Confirmatory factor analyses
- CFI:
-
Comparative fit index
- TLI:
-
Tucker-Lewis index
- RMSEA:
-
Root mean square error of approximation
References
Agishtein, P., & Brumbaugh, C. (2013). Cultural variation in adult attachment: The impact of ethnicity, collectivism, and country of origin. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 7, 384–405. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0099181.
Alonso-Arbiol, I., Balluerka, N., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). A Spanish version of the Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR) adult attachment questionnaire. Personal Relationships, 14, 45–63. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2006.00141.x.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226–244. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.226.
Birnbaum, G. E. (2007). Attachment orientations, sexual functioning, and relationship satisfaction in a community sample of women. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 24, 21–35. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407507072576.
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York, NY: Wiley.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss, Vol. 1.: Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books (Original work published 1969).
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson, & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships, (pp. 46–76). New York: Guilford Press.
Brenning, K. M., & Braet, C. (2013). The emotion regulation model of attachment: An emotion-specific approach. Personal Relationships, 20, 107–123. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2012.01399.x.
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen, & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models, (pp. 136–162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Butzer, B., & Campbell, L. (2008). Adult attachment, sexual satisfaction, and relationship satisfaction: A study of married couples. Personal Relationships, 15, 141–154. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2007.00189.x.
Cameron, J. J., Finnegan, H., & Morry, M. M. (2012). Orthogonal dreams in an oblique world: A meta-analysis of the association between attachment anxiety and avoidance. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 472–476. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2012.05.001.
Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 644–663. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.58.4.644.
Daouk-Öyry, L., & Zeinoun. P. (2017). Testing across cultures: Translation, adaptation and indigenous test development. In B. Cripps (Ed). Psychometric Testing. Critical Perspectives (pp. 221- 234). UK: John Wiley & Sons.
Del Giudice, M. (2011). Sex differences in romantic attachment: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 193–214. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210392789.
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13.
Elliott, S., & Umberson, D. (2008). The performance of desire: Gender and sexual negotiation in long-term marriages. Journal of Marriage and Family, 70, 391–406. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2008.00489.x.
Feeney, J. A. (2016). Adult romantic attachment: Developments in the study of couple relationships. In J. Cassidy, & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications, (3rd ed., pp. 934–957). New York: Guilford.
Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., & Hanrahan, M. (1994). Assessing adult attachment. In M. B. Sperling, & W. H. Berman (Eds.), Attachment in adults: Clinical and developmental perspectives, (pp. 128–152). New York: Guilford Press.
Fernández, A. M., & Dufey, M. (2015). Adaptation of Collins’ Revised Adult Attachment Dimensional Scale to the Chilean context. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 28, 242–252. https://doi.org/10.1590/1678-7153.201528204.
Fraley, R. C., Heffernan, M. E., Vicary, A. M., & Brumbaugh, C. C. (2011). The Experiences in Close Relationships-Relationship Structures Questionnaire: A method for assessing attachment orientations across relationships. Psychological Assessment, 23, 615–625. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022898.
Friedman, M., Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J., Bond, M., Diaz-Loving, R., & Chan, C. (2010). Attachment avoidance and the cultural fit hypothesis: A cross-cultural investigation. Personal Relationships, 17, 107–126. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01256.x.
Gempp, R., Avendaño, C., & Muñoz, C. (2004). Normas y punto de corte para la Escala de Depresión del Centro para Estudios Epidemiológicos (CES-D) en población juvenil chilena [Norms and cutoff point for The Center of Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) in Chilean youthful population]. Terapia Psicológica, 22(2), 145–156.
Gillath, O., Karantzas, G. C., & Fraley, C. (2016). Adult attachment: A concise introduction to theory and research. London: Academic Press.
Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 41–54. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94.
Guzmán, M., & Contreras, P. (2012). Estilos de apego en relaciones de pareja y su asociación con la satisfacción marital [Attachment styles in couples and their association with marital satisfaction]. Psykhe, 21, 69–82. https://doi.org/10.4067/S0718-22282012000100005.
Guzmán-González, M., Barrientos, J., Cárdenas, M., Espinoza, M. F., Quijada, P., Rivera, C., & Tapia, P. (2016a). Romantic attachment and life satisfaction in a sample of gay men and lesbians in Chile. International Journal of Sexual Health, 28, 141–150. https://doi.org/10.1080/19317611.2016.1153014.
Guzmán-González, M., Lafontaine, M.-F., & Levesque, C. (2016b). Romantic attachment and physical intimate partner violence perpetration in a Chilean sample: The mediating role of emotion regulation difficulties. Violence and Victims, 31, 854–868. https://doi.org/10.1891/0886-6708.VV-D-14-00114.
Guzmán-González, M., Péloquin, K., Lafontaine, M.-F., Trabucco, C., & Urzúa, A. (2014). Evaluación de la empatía diádica: Análisis de las propiedades psicométricas del Índice de Reactividad Interpersonal en parejas (IRIC-C) en contexto chileno [Measuring dyadic empathy: Analysis of the psychometrics properties of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index of Couples (IRIC-C) in the Chilean context]. Psicoperspectivas, 13, 156–164. https://doi.org/10.5027/psicoperspectivas-Vol13-Issue2-fulltext-340.
Guzmán-González, M., Trabucco, C., Urzúa, A., Garrido, L., & Leiva, J. (2014). Validez y confiabilidad de la versión adaptada al español de la Escala de Dificultades de Regulación Emocional (DERS-E) en población chilena [Validity and reliability of the adapted Spanish version of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale in Chilean population]. Terapia Psicológica, 32, 19–29. https://doi.org/10.4067/S0718-4808201400010.002.
Hambleton, R. K., Merenda, P. F., & Spielberg, C. D. (2005). Adapting educational and psychological test for cross-cultural assessment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Han, S., & Kahn, J. H. (2017). Attachment, emotion regulation difficulties, and disordered eating among college women and men. The Counseling Psychologist, 45, 1066–1090. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000017744884.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511–524. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.511.
Hendrick, S. S. (1988). A generic measure of relationship satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 50, 93–98. https://doi.org/10.2307/352430.
Heresi-Milad, E., Rivera-Ottenberger, D., & Huepe-Artigas, D. (2014). Associations among attachment, sexuality, and marital satisfaction in adult Chilean couples: A linear hierarchical models analysis. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 40, 259–274. https://doi.org/10.1080/0092623X.2012.756840.
Horne, S. G., & Biss, W. J. (2009). Equality discrepancy between women in same-sex relationships: The mediating role of attachment in relationship satisfaction. Sex Roles, 60, 721–730. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9571-7.
Keller, H. (2013). Attachment and culture. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 44, 175–194. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022112472253.
Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling, (4th ed.,). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Lafontaine, M.-F., Brassard, A., Lussier, Y., Valois, P., Shaver, P. R., & Johnson, S. M. (2016a). Selecting the best items for a short-form of the Experiences in Close Relationships Questionnaire. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 32, 140–154. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000243.
Lafontaine, M.-F., Guzmán-González, M., Péloquin, K., & Levesque, C. (2016b). I am not in your shoes: Low perspective taking mediating the relation among attachment insecurities and physical intimate partner violence in Chilean university students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 24, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260516632812.
Lambert, M. J., Burlingame, G. M., Umphress, V., Hansen, N. B., Vermeersch, D. A., Clouse, G. C., & Yanchar, S. C. (1996). The reliability and validity of the Outcome Questionnaire. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 3, 249-258. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0879(199612)3:4 < 249::AID-CPP106 > 3.0.CO;2-S
Li, T., & Chan, D. K. S. (2012). How anxious and avoidant attachment affect romantic relationship quality differently: A meta-analytic review. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42(4), 406–419. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1842.
Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 335–343. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2016). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change, (2nd ed., ). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Slav, K. (2006). Attachment, mental representations of others, and gratitude and forgiveness in romantic relationships. In M. Mikulincer, & G. S. Goodman (Eds.), Dynamics of romantic love: Attachment, caregiving, and sex, (pp. 190–215). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Molero, F., Shaver, P. R., Fernández, I., Alonso-Arbiol, I., & Recio, P. (2016). Long-term partners’ relationship satisfaction and their perceptions of each other’s attachment insecurities. Personal Relationships, 23, 159–171. https://doi.org/10.1111/pere.12117.
Molero, F., Shaver, P. R., Ferrer, E., Cuadrado, I., & Alonso-Arbiol, I. (2010). Attachment insecurities and interpersonal processes in Spanish couples: A dyadic approach. Personal Relationships, 18, 617–629. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01325.x.
Mortazavizadeh, Z., & Forstmeier, S. (2018). Adult attachment and mental health: The role of emotion regulation. Archives of Psychology, 2(9). https://doi.org/10.31296/aop.v2i9.83.
Pascuzzo, K., Cyr, C., & Moss, E. (2013). Longitudinal association between adolescent attachment, adult romantic attachment, and emotion regulation strategies. Attachment & Human Development, 15, 83–103. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2013.745713.
Péloquin, K., & Lafontaine, M.-F. (2010). Measuring empathy in couples: Validity and reliability of the interpersonal reactivity index for couples. Journal of Personality Assessment, 92, 146–157. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223890903510399.
Péloquin, K., Lafontaine, M.-F., & Brassard, A. (2011). A dyadic approach to the study of romantic attachment, dyadic empathy, and psychological partner aggression. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 28, 915–942. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407510397988.
Peplau, L. A. (2001). Rethinking women’s sexual orientation: An interdisciplinary, relationship-focused approach. Personal Relationships, 8, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2001.tb00025.x.
Peplau, L. A., & Fingerhut, A. W. (2007). The close relationships of lesbians and gay men. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 405–424. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085701.
Radloff, L. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/01466216770010036.
Riggle, E. D. B., Rothblum, E. D., Rostosky, S. S., Clark, J. B., & Balsam, K. F. (2016). “The secret of our success”: Long-term same-sex couples’ perceptions of their relationship longevity. Journal of GLBT Family Studies, 12, 319–334. https://doi.org/10.1080/1550428X.2015.1095668.
Rivera, D., Cruz, C., & Muñoz, C. (2011). Satisfacción en las relaciones de pareja en la adultez emergente: El rol del apego, la intimidad y la depresión [Satisfaction in intimate relationships in emerging adulthood: The role of attachment, intimacy and depression]. Terapia Psicológica, 29, 77–83. https://doi.org/10.4067/s0718-48082011000100008.
Rodríguez-Mosquera, P. M. (2018). Cultural concerns: How valuing social-image shapes social emotion. European Review of Social Psychology, 29, 1–37. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2017.1412180.
Schmitt, D. P. (2010). Romantic attachment from Argentina to Zimbabwe: Patterns of adaptive variation across contexts, cultures, and local ecologies. In P. Erdman, & K.-M. Ng (Eds.), Attachment: Expanding the cultural connections, (pp. 211–226). New York, NY: Routledge.
Simon, R. W., & Nath, L. E. (2004). Gender and motion in the United States: Do men and women differ in self-reports of feelings and expressive behavior? American Journal of Sociology, 109, 1137–1176. https://doi.org/10.1086/382111.
Simpson, J. A. (1990). Influence of attachment styles on romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 971–980. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.5.971.
Spencer, R., Alonso-Arbiol, I., & Fresno, A. (2013a). Romantic attachment in Chilean adults: Development of a short-form version of the Experiences in Close Relationships. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 16, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2013.64.
Spencer, R., Guzmán, M., Fresno, A., & Ramos, N. (2013b). Validación chilena del Cuestionario de evaluación del apego romántico Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR): Análisis de la Validez de Criterio. [A Chilean validation of the Romantic Attachment Questionnaire Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR): Analysis of criterion validity]. Terapia Psicológica, 31, 313–324. https://doi.org/10.4067/S0718-48082013000300006.
Tepeli Temiz, Z., & Comert, I. T. (2018). The relationship between life satisfaction, attachment styles, and psychological resilience in university students. The Journal of Psychiatry and Neurological Sciences, 31, 274–283. https://doi.org/10.5350/dajpn2018310305.
Umberson, D., Thomeer, M. B., & Lodge, A. C. (2015). Intimacy and emotion work in lesbian, gay, and heterosexual relationships. Journal of Marriage and Family, 77, 542–556. https://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12178.
Uskul, A. K., Oyserman, D., & Schwarz, N. (2010). Cultural emphasis on honor, modesty, or self-enhancement: Implications for the survey response process. In M. Braun, B. Edwards, J. Harkness, T. Johnson, L. Lyberg, P. Mohler, et al. (Eds.), Survey methods in multinational, multiregional and multicultural context. NJ: Wiley.
Valor-Segura, I., Expósito, F., & Moya, M. (2009). Desarrollo y validación de la versión española de la Spouse-Specific Dependence Scale. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 9(3), 479–500.
Vera-Villarroel, P., Urzúa, A., Pavez, P., Celis-Atenas, K., & Silva, J. (2012). Evaluation of subjective well-being: Analysis of the Satisfaction with Life Scale in the Chilean population. Universitas Psychologica, 11(3), 719–727.
Vicedo, M. (2017). Putting attachment in its place: Disciplinary and cultural contexts. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 684–699. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2017.1289838.
Vinet, E., Rehbein, L., Román F., & Saiz, J. (2008). Escalas abreviadas de depresión, ansiedad y estrés (DASS-21). Versión chilena traducida y adaptada. Unpublished manuscript, Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco, Chile.
von Bergen, A., & de la Parra, G. (2002). OQ-45.2, Cuestionario para evaluación de resultados y evolución en psicoterapia: Adaptación, validación e indicaciones para su aplicación e interpretación [OQ-45.2, an Outcome Questionnaire for monitoring change in psychotherapy: Adaptation, validation and indications for its application and interpretation]. Revista Terapia Psicológica, 20(2), 161–176.
Wei, M., Liao, K. Y.-H., Ku, T.-Y., & Shaffer, P. A. (2011). Attachment, self-compassion, empathy, and subjective well-being among college students and community adults. Journal of Personality, 79, 191–221. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00677.x.
Wei, M., Russell, D. W., Mallinckrodt, B., & Vogel, D. L. (2007). The Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR) Scale-short form: Reliability, validity, and factor structure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 88, 187–204. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223890701268041.
Zakalik, R. A., & Wei, M. (2006). Adult attachment, perceived discrimination based on sexual orientation, and depression in gay males: Examining the mediation and moderation effects. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 302–313. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.3.302.
Acknowledgements
Not applicable
Funding
This research was partially supported by the Chilean National Fund for Scientific and Technological Development Fondecyt 1140887, and by CONICYT (National Fund for Research and Technology), Grant N° PII PII20150035, with co-funding of the Fund for Innovation and Competitiveness (FIC) of the Chilean Ministry of Economy, Development and Tourism, through the Millennium Scientific Initiative, Grant N° IS130005
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
MG-G designed and executed the study and wrote the paper. DR-O collaborated in the writing of the paper and part of the discussion; AB analyzed the data and wrote the results. RS collaborated in the writing of the paper and the editing of the final manuscript; MF-L collaborated in the writing of the paper and the editing of the final manuscript. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Guzmán-González, M., Rivera-Ottenberger, D., Brassard, A. et al. Measuring adult romantic attachment: psychometric properties of the brief Spanish version of the experiences in close relationships. Psicol. Refl. Crít. 33, 9 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-020-00145-w
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-020-00145-w